The Gender Gap in Math and Math-Heavy Fields: a Statistical Overview
Countless studies have debunked the notion of gender-based intelligence differences in mathematics, showing that girls are just as good as boys. Yet, the current state of affairs remains unsatisfactory, with a significant gender disparity prevailing across educational and professional landscapes. The shocking statistics are hard to ignore:
In 2021, women comprise only 28% of the science and engineering workforce
In 2020, only 25% of bachelor’s degrees in math, computer science, and engineering were earned by women
Women are 1.5 times more likely than men to leave the STEM pipeline after taking Calculus I.
Every year, girls score around 20 points lower on SAT math than boys do.
Gender Stereotyping as the Root Cause of Gender Divide in Math
Such a substantial gender gap in math and math-heavy fields can be traced back to gender stereotypes. Girls as young as 6 and 7 years old internalize the prejudicial belief that girls are less capable of math than boys. In one study, when elementary school girls were asked to draw a mathematician, the girls were twice as likely to draw a man (61%) than a woman (30.5%). Furthermore, the cultural belief associating “math brains” with boys has undermined girls’ confidence and sense of belonging in math. In one study, despite having similar grades, middle school girls reported considerably lower self-esteem and self-efficacy in their math abilities than boys did. In a high school student survey, only 58% of girls described their math and science class participation as “frequent and confident,” compared to 76% of their male counterparts.
The Power of Mindset in Overcoming Gender Stereotypes in Math
Now, you might wonder, given the near impossibility of eliminating gender bias from society, can we ever imagine a world where all girls confidently realize their math potential? The answer is yes. Gender stereotyping may threaten girls’ self-assurance and performance in math, but the differentiating factor between those who give in to it and those who don’t is mindset. In two studies, one conducted on Columbia students and another on high school 11th-graders, those who held a growth mindset–perceiving their math proficiency as something they could strengthen through effort–were unaffected by gender bias both academically and psychologically. Unconvinced by stereotypical beliefs, girls with a growth mindset embraced challenging mathematical tasks, maintained their motivation in the face of difficulties, and actively learned from the mistakes they made when solving math problems. In contrast, stereotyping strongly impacted girls who held a fixed mindset–viewing their math ability as innate and unchangeable. When bumping up against a frustrating task, girls with a fixed mindset often doubted their competence in math; gradually, many of them no longer felt included and comfortable in math settings.
Returning to the earlier question, how can we shatter the barriers that have held back our girls in math? While urging society members to stop labeling girls in math is vital, equally important is transforming girls’ perceptions of such stereotypes. And how do we ignite this change? Through nurturing a growth mindset in girls.
References
Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G. (2014). Girls and Women in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732214549471
Dweck, C. S. (2007). Is Math a Gift? Beliefs That Put Females at Risk. In S. J. Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why aren't more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 47–55). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11546-004
Ellis, J., Fosdick, B. K., & Rasmussen, C. (2016). Women 1.5 Times More Likely to Leave STEM Pipeline after Calculus Compared to Men: Lack of Mathematical Confidence a Potential Culprit. PloS One, 11(7), e0157447. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157447
Grieco, E. (2023). Diversity and STEM: Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities 2023. NSF - National Science Foundation. https://ncses.nsf.gov/pubs/nsf23315/report/acknowledgments-and-citation#acknowledgments-and-citation
Herbert, J., & Stipek, D. (2005). The emergence of gender differences in children's perceptions of their academic competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 276–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.02.007
Nam, J. (2023). Average SAT Scores: Full Statistics. BestColleges.Com.
Picker, S. H., & Berry, J. S. (2000). Investigating pupils’ images of mathematicians. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 43(1), 65–94. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017523230758
Song, S., Jiang, N., & Ji, L. (2022). ERIC - EJ1366241 - Growth Mindset Training and the Effect of Math-Gender Stereotype Threat on Girl Students, Best Evidence in Chinese Education, 2022. Best Evidence in Chinese Education, 12(2), 1629–1634.
Travers, M. (2020). Even Among Elite Students, Boys Are More Confident In Math Than Girls. Forbes. https://www.forbes.com/sites/traversmark/2020/04/09/even-among-elite-students-boys-are-more-confident-in-math-than-girls/?sh=a7f6b757200f
Zander, L., Höhne, E., Harms, S., Pfost, M., & Hornsey, M. J. (2020). When Grades Are High but Self-Efficacy Is Low: Unpacking the Confidence Gap Between Girls and Boys in Mathematics. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 552355. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552355
Comentários